Here are the notes and slides for a recent presentation on placebo effects in veterinary medicine.
WHAT IS A PLACEBO?
Despite the fact that most people have heard of the placebo effect and feel they have some understanding of what it means, the concept is a complex and contentious one. There is no consensus definition of a placebo or of the placebo effect. However, it is generally accepted that placebos are inert, and they have no direct physiologic effect on a patient’s disease or symptoms. Placebo effects, then, are effects associated with the administration of an inert substance or treatment, and these effects are perceived as beneficial by the patient or observers. Negative effects associated with inert treatments do occur, and these are referred to as nocebo effects.1
There are a number of mechanisms by which an inert treatment can lead to reported or observed responses. Many placebo effects are psychological responses based on belief and expectation, which influence a patient’s perception of his or her symptoms or have physiologic effects through centrally mediated changes in autonomic function.1-2 Placebo effects may also be caused by classical conditioning, in which physiologic responses to active treatments are paired with inert stimuli that are eventually able to elicit the same responses as the active treatment.2
Some effects associated with inert treatments in clinical research studies are actually not placebo effects mediated by conditioning or expectations. Research subjects in a placebo control group may experiences changes in their symptoms or other outcomes for a variety of reasons, and the use of an inert treatment can reveal that these changes are not due to the active treatment. However, not all such changes are true placebo effects.
For example, the Hawthorne Effect is a phenomenon in which subjects improve simply as a consequence of being enrolled in a research study.3 Subjects get more care and attention, they tend to be more compliant with existing treatments because they are being monitored, and their condition may improve due to better overall care regardless of any active test treatment or expectation-based placebo effects.3
Regression to the mean and the natural course of disease are other phenomena that can be seen in subjects receiving placebos in a research study which is not actually a true placebo effect.4 With chronic conditions, symptoms tend to wax and wane spontaneously. And some conditions may resolve spontaneously Patients are more likely to seek care, or enroll in research studies, when their symptoms are waxing, and thus they tend to improve after receiving care or beginning a study due to the natural course of disease. This, again, can be revealed by placebo assignment, but it is not an effect of the administration of the inert treatment.
It is worth noting that all of the factors that cause placebo effects and apparent response to inert treatment can also be seen with active treatments. Pain relievers with direct physiologic activity can elicit a greater reported response than explained by the pharmacological effects of the treatment. Part of the purpose of inert treatments in research trials is not only to assess whether treatments have specific effects but what proportion of the apparent response may be explained by concurrent non-specific effects and placebo responses.
In general use, placebo effects refer to those changes in reported or measured symptoms associated with beliefs or expectations about treatment. However, in the context of medical research, any effect seen in the placebo control group is classified as a placebo effect even if it is due to other factors.1 This complicates the clear use of the term and an understanding of what effects can be attributed to inert treatments and the associated mechanisms. It also impacts the relevance of placebo effects to veterinary patients, in which the relative role of various causes for non-specific treatment effects likely differ from human patients.
PLACEBO EFFECTS IN ANIMALS
It is clear that animal subjects in research studies exhibit changes in symptoms commonly identified as placebo effects when seen in human research subjects. These include subjective symptoms reported by owners or observed by investigators and clinicians as well as objectively measured outcomes.2,5-8 There is no clear research evidence showing such effects in clinical veterinary patients not participating in research. However, the circumstances and variables responsible for placebo effects seen in veterinary research studies are usually also present in the clinical context, so it is likely such effects exist in clinical patients as well as research subjects.
Animals receiving inert treatments often show improvement in subjective outcomes, such as pain, which are assessed by caregivers, clinicians or researchers. While it is generally accepted that most animals are not cognitively capable of having beliefs and expectations about their medical care, and so cannot have the classic direct placebo response, caregivers and other humans involved in these studies are susceptible to such effects. The caregiver placebo effect, in which humans report improvement in subjective symptoms for animals receiving inert treatment, has been clearly demonstrated.5,7
It is also likely that animals receiving inert treatments may show improvement due to causes other than direct placebo effects. Research has shown human contact has predictable effects on behavioral and physiologic responses in domesticated animals, and these effects can easily be interpreted as improvement in clinical symptoms.2,9 And just as classical condition contributes to placebo effects in humans, such conditioned responses are certainly present in other animals and likely generate changes in the condition of animal patients and research subjects receiving inert treatments.
Even relatively objective outcome measures have been shown to change in response to the administration of inert treatments. Seizure frequency, heart rate, and other objectively measurable outcomes show non-specific treatment effects in animals just as in humans.2,8 Conditioning, the Hawthorne Effect, general response to human contact, and other variables are likely responsible for these apparent placebo effects.
CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS
The presence of placebo effects and other responses to inert treatments has several important implications for clinical care. One is that apparent responses to treatment, especially in subjectively experienced symptoms such as pain, nausea, and fatigue, may reflect placebo effects rather than true improvement in the underlying condition. Human asthma patients, for example, may report improvements in the symptoms experienced during an asthma attack when given a placebo inhaler.10 However, such responses are not typically seen in objective signs of disease, especially in patients not in clinical trials receiving more intense and comprehensive monitoring and care than clinical patients. For example, the asthma patients who reported feeling better with placebo inhalers had no measurable improvement in lung function, unlike those patients receiving bronchodilator therapy.10
This demonstrates that placebo effects generally improve the perception of symptoms but not the actual physical disease. Such effects can fool patients, caregivers, and clinicians into believing they have provided effective treatment while allowing the disease to remain of progress. Asthma patients consistently treated with an ineffective medication would likely perceive some relief, but they could also be experiencing ongoing lung damage and ultimately have poorer outcomes due to the lack of objectively effective treatment. It is critical, then, that clinical therapies be demonstrated to be truly effective through clinical trial research because uncontrolled clinical observation is an unreliable guide to efficacy.
There are also ethical implications to placebo effects.11-12 In human medicine, there may be some benefit to improving the perception of uncomfortable symptoms, with a placebo or with placebo effects attached to active treatments. However, such effects are typically negligible if patients are informed that they are receiving inert treatments. Obtaining the subjective benefits of placebo effects requires misleading patients into believing they are receiving an active therapy, which is arguably unethical.11
In veterinary patients, placebo effects are largely obtained by proxy through caregivers.5,7 This creates an additional ethical challenge since caregivers and clinicians may perceive benefits the patients are not actually experiencing. This makes the necessity of objective research validation for treatment efficacy even more critical in veterinary medicine.
CONCLUSIONS
Placebo effects, and other related factors that create true or perceived improvement in clinical symptoms, are manifest in veterinary patients and animal research subjects. Controlling for these effects in research studies is crucial to identifying the true benefits of the treatments we employ. The existence of such effects also makes clinical observation of response to therapy highly unreliable as a measure of the true efficacy of our treatments. We, and our clients, often see what we hope or expect to see in response to the therapies we employ, and it is possible to be fooled into believing ineffective treatments are working without appropriate controlled research evidence. This creates an ethical imperative to base our interventions on good-quality research evidence rather than uncontrolled observations. Fortunately, some kinds of non-specific effects seen with inert treatments can also add to the real benefits of active treatments, and we can take advantage of this to maximize the benefits of our therapies once we have established objective efficacy.
REFERENCES
- De Crae AJM. Kaptchuk TJ. Tjissen JGP. et al. Placebos and placebo effects in medicine: historical overview. J Royal Soc Med. 1999;92:511-15.
- MacMillan FD. The placebo effect in animals. J Amer Vet Med Assoc. 1999;215(7):992-9.
- McCarney R. Warner J. Iliffe S. et al. The Hawthorne Effect: a randomised, controlled trial. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2007;7(30).
- McDonald CJ. Mazzuca SA. Mcabe PG. How much of the placebo ‘effect’ is really statistical regression? Statistics Med. 1983;2:417-27.
- Conzemius MG. Evans RB. Caregiver placebo effect for dogs with lameness from osteoarthritis. J Am Vet Med Assoc. 2012;241(10):1314-9.
- Malek S. Sample SJ. Schwartz Z. et al. Effect of analgesic therapy on clinical outcome measures in a randomized controlled trial using client-owned dogs with hip osteoarthritis. BMC Vet Res. 2012;4(8):185.
- Gruen ME. Dorman DC. Lascelles BDX. Caregiver placebo effect in analgesic clinical trials for cats with naturally occurring degenerative joint disease-associated pain. Vet Rec. 2017;180(19):473.
- Muñana KR. Zhang D. Patterson EE. Placebo effect in canine epilepsy trials. J Vet Intern Med. 2010;24(1):166-70.
- Zulkifli I. Review of human-animal interactions and their impact on animal productivity and welfare. J Anim Sci Biotech. 2013;4(1):25.
- Wechsler ME. Kelley JM. Boyd IOE. Et al. Active albuterol or placebo, sham acupuncture, or no intervention in asthma. N Engl J Med 2011;365:119-126.
- Asai A. Kadooka Y. Reexamination of the ethics of placebo use in clinical practice. Bioethics.2013;27(4):186-93.
- Gold A. Lichtenberg P. The moral case for the clinical placebo. J Med Ethics. 2014;40(4):219-24.
Placebos in Veterinary Medicine Slides
Pingback: Articles I’ve Enjoyed Recently | Little Brown Dog Blog