Nutraceuticals & Cognitive Dysfunction–An Update

Back in September, I reviewed the evidence (or lack thereof) supporting claims of benefits from nutraceuticals in cognitive dysfunction. One of the more popular ingredients in many of the products sold with such claims is Ginkgo biloba, and at the time of my earlier review I concluded:

“The clinical trial evidence in humans does not support a beneficial effect for cognitive impairment or dementia, as summarized in a Cochrane review…”

This month, a new study of ginkgo biloba for prevention of cognitive decline in humans has been published in the Journal of the American Medical Association. It is a large, multicenter, randomized, double-blinded, placebo-controlled prospective clinical trial, and thus represents a very high level of evidence. It concludes quite clearly.\

“Compared with placebo, the use of G biloba, 120 mg twice daily, did not result in less cognitive decline in older adults with normal cognition or with mild cognitive impairment.”

Another nail in the coffin for this particular wonder-herb.

Posted in Herbs and Supplements | 1 Comment

Exercise in Puppies-Are there rules?

There are many dogmatic opinions available from veterinarians, pet owners, breeders and others concerning a common question owners of new puppies have, How much exercise is ok for puppies? This is an especially pertinent question for owners of large breed puppies, since these breeds have a higher incidence than others of developmental orthopedic disorders such as hip dysplasia, elbow dysplasia, and cartilage abnormalities known as osteochondrosis dissecans (OCD). As is all too often the case, however, these opinions generally lack solid scientific evidence to support them. Very little is known about the precise risks and benefits of different types and intensities of exercise in growing animals.

One case control observational study [1] surveyed dog owners and found playing with other dogs to be a risk factor for OCD. Another, similar study [2] found chasing balls and sticks was a risk factor for development of hip dysplasia and elbow abnormalities. However, these studies cannot answer the overall question, which is how much and what kinds of exercise pose how great a risk and provide how great a benefit. One study [3] found exercise to be part of the treatment of carpal laxity, another joint abnormality seen in large breed puppies, and there is no question that exercise has many benefits, including reducing the risk of obesity and simply being part of a normal, enjoyable life for a puppy.

There are many more studies on the effects of exercise in children than in puppies, and though it is always risky to extrapolate from one species to another, some useful information can be gained by using one organism as a model for another, as long as conclusions drawn in this way are cautious and tentative pending better data. In general, while some intense and repetitive exercise can pose a risk of damage to growth plates in children, exercise is overall seen as beneficial in improving bone density and reducing the risk of obesity and related health problems.

The research evidence, then, really does not provide anything like a definitive answer to questions about the effects of exercise in growing puppies. Common sense suggests that forcing a dog to exercise heavily when it does not wish to is not a good idea. Likewise, puppies sometimes have more enthusiasm than sense and can exercise to the point of heat exhaustion, blistered footpads, and other damage that may be less obvious. Therefore, a general principle of avoiding forced or voluntary extreme exercise is reasonable, but specific and absolute statements about what kind of exercise is allowed, what surfaces puppies should or should not exercise on, and so forth is merely opinion not supported by objective data. Such opinions may very well be informed by personal experience, and they may be reliable, but any opinion not founded on objective data must always be taken with a grain of salt and accepted provisionally until such data is available.

1. Slater MR, Scarlett JM, Donoghue S, Kaderly RE, Bonnett BN, Cockshutt J, et al. Diet and exercise as potential risk factors for osteochondritis dissecans in dogs. Am J Vet Res. 1992 Nov;53(11):2119-24.

2. Sallander MH, Hedhammar A, Trogen ME. Diet, exercise, and weight as risk factors in hip dysplasia and elbow arthrosis in Labrador Retrievers. J Nutr. 2006 Jul;136(7 Suppl):2050S-2052S.

3. Cetinkaya MA, Yardimci C, Sa?lam M. Carpal laxity syndrome in forty-three puppies.Vet Comp Orthop Traumatol. 2007;20(2):126-30.

Posted in Science-Based Veterinary Medicine | 27 Comments

Short and Sour–Politics & Fear Trump Evidence

Much virtual ink has been spilled over the United Stated Preventative Services Task Force (USPSTF) breast cancer screening recommendations. Though I’ve written in general terms about the generally underappreciated complexity of screening tests and whether they are the unadulterated good most people think, the specifics of the USPTF recommendations and ensuing controversy have been covered at Science-Based Medicine and by Orac at Respectful Insolence with great detail and insight, so I have not felt motivated to contribute to the discussion surrounding them. However, the aspect of the brouhaha that strikes me as most disturbing and most central to the core issues I deal with here at SkeptVet blog, how to improve the practice of medicine through better use and reliance on science and scientific evidence, have recently been expressed succinctly on the KevinMD Blog. The post is short and to the point, so I will quote it in its entirety with all emphasis being mine:

“The fallout from the mammogram screening guidelines have served as a test case, of sorts, to see how the politicians and public will respond to recommendations based on evidence-based clinical practice.

And, judging from the inflammatory reaction, it’s safe to say that we’re quite a ways from medical decisions based on the best available data.

In a recent editorial, the New York Times touched upon the issue. One of the Senate’s health care bill amendments explicitly mentioned the USPSTF and “directed the government to ignore the task force’s most recent mammography recommendations.”

It overwhelmingly passed.

Health reformers are hoping that results from comparative effectiveness trials can help reduce the amount of practice variation, which is a leading driver of rising health spending.

But whenever the evidence calls for less medicine, the political and public outcry will be deafening. Today it’s mammograms. What if tomorrow an independent body calls for, say, a reduction of angioplasty or cardiac bypass surgery, which studies have suggested are being overused?

Both the politicians and the public will simply cry, “Rationing!” Thus, the myth that more care is better care will continue to be perpetuated, and the data ignored.

I strive to avoid cynicism in my outlook on the future of science-based medicine, but I have to agree wholeheartedly with these sentiments. Orac has illustrated flaws and errors in the way the recommendations were marketed to the public, but while he may well be right, I think it is beside the point. Fear and a natural risk aversion makes us predisposed to make bad judgments about risk and benefit and to always seek what appears superficially like the safest course even when the data shows it is not the most likely to lead to a good outcome. It is difficult for doctors to do nothing even when that is the right thing to do, and it is even more difficult to convince patients and clients that it is sometimes better not to intervene with a test or treatment. Primum non nocere (First do no harm) is a cliché, but despite the fact we’ve all heard it, we seem constitutionally challenged when it comes to heading it. Hmmm, maybe if we printed it on T-shirts and gave  them away to doctors? Seems to work for the pharmaceutical reps. Or perhaps we simply have to give up on the current generation and focus on teaching critical thinking starting in pre-school, inculcating the habit of ignoring one’s own intuition when the data says we should? Any ideas?

Posted in Law, Regulation, and Politics | 1 Comment

No CAM Degrees in Israel

I stumbled across an article in the Israeli newspaper Haaraetz indicating that Council for Higher Education, the national accrediting body for higher education, has declined to permit complementary and alternative medicine programs to offer academic degrees. According to the article:

“[the Council] rejected several requests on the matter, saying there was no scientific basis for the practice…’Almost all studies show clearly that there is no scientific basis for alternative medicine,’ the council said yesterday.

While conceding that the various health maintenance organizations operate extensive alternative medicine treatment programs, they said ‘from that to academization is a very long way.'”

What a refreshing application of reason and science to a policy decision. The council recognized that CAM is widely practiced in Israel but rejected the argument of a subcommittee that it is better to legitimize it by accrediting the CAM education programs in order to somehow control the “quality” of the CAM offered. That argument makes little sense since it is pointless to ensure only the highest quality nonsense can be granted the imprimatur of a college degree. A PhD in astrology is just as meaningless as an unaccredited degree in the subject.

It would be nice to imagine such a level of common sense could one day apply to the accreditation process in smaller, less developed nations with less sophisticated educational systems, like the U.S. But I don’t imagine we’ll see that any time soon.

Posted in Law, Regulation, and Politics | Leave a comment

Why Incompatible CAM Theories Aren’t

I am often struck by what I consider one of the great mysteries of CAM: how theories and approaches that are mutually inconsistent and incompatible are unified under the CAM umbrella without any apparent notice being taken of their mutually exclusive dogmas. The American Holistic Veterinary Medical Association (AAVMA), which I’ve written about before, is the paragon of this phenomenon. The organization supports and publishes “research” articles about almost any quack therapy without discrimination, so long as it cannot be said to be solidly based on scientific research evidence. The siege mentality seems to blind members of the CAM community to their internal differences.

Chiropractic is based on the theory that a mystical energy called innate intelligence travels through the spine and that subtle misalignments of the vertebrae (subluxations) block the normal flow of this energy, which leads to all disease. Granted, some chiropractors treat these ideas much like many Christians treat the Bible, as metaphors rather than literal truth, but the idea is still the foundation of the method and still widely invoked, especially since those chiropractors who have renounced it are left rather lamely claiming that their method works because people say it works even if no one can say why.

Traditional acupuncture, insofar as it has any consistent theoretical foundation, relies on the similarly vitalistic concept of qi which gets blocked or unbalanced to cause all disease. Manipulation of the qi as it flows through meridians by penetration with needles is the key element of acupuncture therapy. Again, some acupuncturists have attempted to jettison the vitalism underlying the tradition of their approach, though they haven’t come up with a demonstrable, consistent alternative explanation of how acupuncture might work, but many still employ the traditional concepts and language.

Homeopathy is based on another vitalist force gone awry, only this time the key to treatment of all disease is identifying a natural substance that induces the same symptoms as a particular disorder (Law of Similars) and then making it into a potent medicine by diluting and shaking it (Law of Infinitesimals and succussion). The contortions of homeopaths to try and rationalize this nonsense by bizarre interpretations of quantum mechanics are especially entertaining.

Many less popular CAM approaches rely on similar vitalist concepts, and yet many, many of them claim a unitary cause of disease and a single method for treatment, and no one seems to notice that all of these theories and all of these treatments cannot all be the one true cause/treatment of disease. Yet it is quite common for practitioners and patients alike to combine methods and recommend several or all of them. Clearly, the reality is that whatever unites these folks, it is not a particular theory nor even a commitment to their various one true answers. So what is it?

Well, epistemologically of course it is the belief that personal experience and intuition are reliable and sufficient to confirm the truth of an idea or the efficacy of a therapy. There is a strong tendency to a post-modernist relativism that suggests multiple incompatible truths can all be true for someone somewhere since there is no single physical reality out there waiting to be understood, as methodological naturalism and most scientific epistemology assumes.

Politically, CAM practitioners share a sense of being bold and unconventional thinkers surrounded by an oppressive and unimaginative orthodoxy marching in lockstep and suppressing all dissent. The reality is quite different, as CAM has become a well-financed and politically powerful lobby, but the image still persists in the minds of many practitioners and users.

CAM proponents often view the world as “toxic” and see the benefits of technology and science as hiding malign and poisonous underbellies. They tend to view history as a decline from a romanticized golden age in which humans lived in a state of nature and were healthier and happier to a time of decay in which science serves only to abuse and oppress the earth and the body.

There are undoubtedly other philosophical and aesthetic commonalities to the CAM movement, and I think it would be a fascinating topic for detailed study. Unfortunately, the general distrust of the very notion that there is a single objective reality that exists for everyone everywhere and that isn’t influenced at all by our beliefs or feelings makes possible a kind of doublethink in which mutually inconsistent medical theories can all be true. This makes it possible to suggest with a straight face that CAM might really be “complementary” even to scientific medical practice while still rejecting the very philosophical and epistemological premises that underlie science-based medicine. It would be nice if pointing out the logical inconsistency here had an impact on the beliefs of CAM followers, but they are well-protected against such reasoning.

Posted in General | 1 Comment

Homeopathy in the ER

Posted in Humor | Leave a comment

Who Says Skeptics Aren’t Fun?

A 9-minute beat poem of rationalism and skepticism. Who says skeptics aren’t funny?! 🙂

Posted in Humor | 3 Comments

The End of Chiropractic? Of Course Not.

At last, the definitive nail in the coffin of chiropractic? Hardly. A recent study in the journal Chiropractic and Osteopathy uses an epidemiological approach to examine the question of whether the founding “lesion” of the chiropractic philosophy, the vertebral subluxation, can be shown to be associated with any disease. Their conclusion reads like a Science-Based Medicine or Quackwatch summary:

“Subluxation was not found to be linked to any one disease complex…There were no studies that offered a biological plausibility that would isolate subluxation as a causal factor in disease. There were no studies linking the subluxation as a coherent construct and supported by generally known facts about the natural history and biology of any disease. There were no studies found that suggested the subluxation as a causal agent similar to other factually demonstrated causal agents…There is significant lack of evidence to fulfill basic criteria of causation. This lack of crucial supportive epidemiologic evidence prohibits the accurate promulgation of the chiropractic subluxation.”

Short and sweet, the basic abnormality chiropractors claim to fix cannot be shown to exist or to cause any disease. One might think this would be a fatal blow to the enterprise of chiropractic, but sadly that is not the case. For one thing, it has been shown before in other studies that the subluxation cannot be reliably identified by chiropractors and that when pushed to demonstrate it in some objective way, most chiropractors and their lobbying organizations engage in some impressive yoga to cover the fact that they cannot. But as chiropractic is fundamentally faith-based medicine, being unable to demonstrate that the disease they are treating exists doesn’t worry chiropractors. Even those who disavow the subluxation theory happily continue the sort of manipulative practices Palmer originally invented to fix the supposed subluxation, and they simply justify it with the time-honored refuge of all woo, “Well, I don’t know how or why it works, but it works!”

Of course, this conclusion cannot be reliably demonstrated by objective evidence either. Though chiropractic does seem to have some benefit on subjective perceptions idiopathic lower back pain, the fact is it has been shown to be at best roughly equivalent to conventional therapy (rest, NSAIDs, physical therapy, and patient education materials) or to a good massage. If it were truly risk free and presented truthfully as offering mild benefits for back pain, I would have no objections. And a few chiropractors have adopted this approach in an attempt to take what benefit their interventions might provide out of the realm of faith and into that of responsible medicine, but these are a minority and often reviled within their own profession. But while I believe evidence is critical and should be the cornerstone of medical practice, I also understand that it is not as compelling as personal experience for many people, and the first step on the road to recovery from addiction to unscientific therapies is to understand we have a problem and need something better than our own experience and intuition to validate or invalidate the safety and efficacy of medicine. So while this should be a critical study undermining the claims of mainstream chiropractic, it has garnered little attention and will probably have little impact on the popularity of the method.

Posted in Chiropractic | 2 Comments

Benefits & Risks of Neutering–What does the science say?

Many pet owners have questions about neutering their pet dogs and cats, and while there is much useful information available on the Internet, there is also much misinformation. I have composed a literature review looking at the benefits and risks of spaying and neutering. It is too large to post directly here, so I have uploaded it as a .pdf file on SkeptVet.com. I hope also to soon have a simplified, tabular version for those disinclined to wade through the detailed consideration of all the pros and cons.

http://www.skeptvet.com/web_documents/NeuterProsCons.pdf

Posted in Science-Based Veterinary Medicine | 3 Comments

Homeopathy Works for Arthritis–Or Maybe Not

I recently ran across an article in the journal Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine (eCAM) titled Evaluating Complementary Therapies for Canine Osteoarthritis–Part II: A homeopathic Combination Preparation (Zeel) (Hielm-Bjorkman, A et al 2009:6(4)465-471).

According to the authors, “a homeopathic combination product (HCP) for canine osteoarthritic pain was evaluated in a randomized, double-controlled and double-blinded clinical trial…[and] that the HCP (Zeel) was beneficial in alleviating chronic orthopedic pain in dogs, although it was not as effective as Carprofen.”

There are many levels on which any clinical research article should be critically evaluated. The potential biases of the authors and the journal, the quality of the methodology, the statistical analysis of the data, and the degree to which the conclusions follow from the data are all common criteria by which such publications can be judged. R. Barker Bausell in his book Snake Oil Science does an outstanding job illustrating some reasons why not everything that makes its way into a scientific journal is reliable science and why such critical evaluation is necessary.

In this case, the journal makes some effort to follow the principles of evidence-based medicine, but it is guilty of some serious Tooth Fairy Science, in which rigorous methodology is applied to some fundamentally irrational premises. Skimming through some archival issues also indicates a pretty strong preference for publishing positive findings for CAM interventions. None of this automatically invalidates anything published, but it is one factor to consider since the effect of personal as well as financial biases on research outcomes is well established.

As for the authors, I am not able to establish much about their biases. Two of the authors are professors at the University of Helsinki School of Veterinary Medicine, and the lead author’s research summary  suggests a strong attachment to CAM. Her doctoral dissertation was a study on gold implantation, green-lipped mussel extract, and Zeel for use in canine osteoarthritis, and it appears she is publishing this thesis research as a series of articles in eCAM.

The methodology is generally sound, with a couple of exceptions. First, while the product studied is identified as homeopathic and an inject able version of it is listed in the US Homeopathic Pharmacopoeia, even the authors insert the caveat that “this is not a classical homeopathic treatment.” The preparation contains 14 listed ingredients, many of which are present after having been diluted 1:10 only 2-8 times, for “molar concentrations of 10-5 to 10-12mol/L.” Such concentrations are low, but still higher than the usual case for homeopathic preparations, which cannot conceivable contain any of the original ingredient. It is possible, then, that this substance could contain some pharmacologically active substances. The ingredients listed (same as the injectable product) are:

 

  1. Arnica montana, radix (mountain arnica)
    Dulcamara (bittersweet)
    Rhus toxicodendron (poison oak)
    Sanguinaria canadensis (blood root)
    Symphytum officinale (comfrey)
  2. Mineral ingredients:
    Sulphur (sulphur)
    (alpha)-Lipoicum acid (thioctic acid)
    Coenzyme A (coenzyme A)
    Nadidum (nicotinamide adenine dinucleotide)
    Natrum oxalaceticum (sodium oxalacetate)   
  3. Animal-derived ingredients
    Cartilago suis (porcine cartilage)
    Embryo totalis suis (porcine embryo)
    Funiculus umbilicalis suis (porcine umbilical cord)
    Placenta suis (porcine placenta)

The subjects were appropriately randomized into treatment, placebo, and positive control groups, with Carprofen as the positive control. The subjects in each group all appear to be comparable at baseline. The placebo group did have higher baseline scores on 5/7 measures of pain, but the authors state that no statistically significant differences were found between groups at this point.

The placebo control was not ideal. The treatment product was visibly different from the Carprofen and the placebo (which were identical to each other). The owners were given extra Carprofen in its original packaging for rescue, so clearly they would be able to identify the treatment product as different. In addition, all subjects also received an inert capsule as part of a separate study, so while the Zeel group received “an ampoule of clear liquid” once daily and “a slightly green (lactose) capsule,” the Carprofen and placebo groups received the green capsule and “a white pill” twice daily. It is not clear what if any affect such a discrepancy might have had on the subjective assessments of owners, or potentially blinded investigators who might have detected group assignment from comments made by owners.

Most of the assessment measures were subjective, such as owner rating scales or visual analog pain scores and investigator clinical exam assessment. Some force plate analysis was done, though this proved problematic. Two subjects had to have their force plate measurements discarded because they were too lame to allow accurate measurement. These subjects, however, were all in the placebo arm and so this would be expected to have the effect of decreasing perceived efficacy of the treatment.

The force plate measurements were “repeated until sufficient valid results were obtained for both left and right limbs.” It was not stated whether the number of trials needed to achieve this differed between groups, which could have affected the results is some subjects had to run back and forth significant more than others to get a valid reading, which might itself affect the reading.

The biggest methodological problem I see in the study is in order to calculate the percent of subjects improved or not improved in each group at the 8-week assessment period, “the results of each variable were converted into dichotomous responses of ‘improved’ or ‘not improved.'” Converting scale variables into dichotomous variables can exaggerate differences between groups. If the measurement was unchanged, the subject would be classified as “not improved,” but if there was even a miniscule change from baseline then the subject would fall into either “improved” or not “improved category.” Thus, subjects with dramatic improvements in scale measurements would be weighted the same as subjects with marginal, and likely clinically insignificant changes in the variable. Without the raw data, of course, it is impossible to tell what if any effect this procedure might have had on the final conclusions. However, the tabulation of the data presented in the article appears to show much greater improvement in terms of the percentage of subjects improved than in terms of the actual median improvement of the variables themselves, suggesting that in fact such an exaggeration occurred.

The authors also stated that “for dogs that had used extra Carprofen more than three times per week at W8 [4 dogs in the placebo arm] we changed all their variable values at evaluation W8 into the most negative value measured at that time.…to counteract the effect of the NSAID…” This seems a clear fudging of the data which made the placebo group appear to have worse outcome measures than it actually did. Certainly, it is possible that these dogs needed more Carprofen than the treatment group because the treatment was having a beneficial effect. But it is just as possible that the placebo group took more Carprofen because of differences in disease severity, in owner attitudes or behavior, or some other factor. And what makes the arbitrary designation of three times a week an appropriate justification for altering the data in this way is unclear. In any case, the effect of this decision is to make the outcomes appear worse for the placebo group, which clearly makes the treatment group outcomes appear relatively better.

Patients given Carprofen clearly showed improvement over baseline at a rate significantly higher than placebo. 67-86% of subjects were categorized as “improved” for the various outcome measures, and the actual values for each measure were improved from 2-5 times more than for the Zeel group. In the treatment group significantly more subjects were classed as “improved” compared to placebo in 3 out of 6 measures. Again, this is likely inflated by the conversion of scale data to dichotomous data. The Zeel group also showed significantly greater improvement than the placebo group in 4 out of 6 specific measures, though for one the P value was 0.049, quite close to the cutoff of 0.05.  

The authors also state that use of supplemental or rescue Carprofen occurred in 14% of the Carprofen group, 28% of the Zeel group, and 8% of the placebo group. Though they claim that the only significant difference was between the Carprofen and placebo groups, this is puzzling, both because the Zeel group had a percent of rescue use dramatically higher than the other groups, and because of the earlier statement about manipulating the data for the placebo group to “counteract” the effect of Carprofen use for that group.

No differences in bloodwork values or clinical side effects were seen between groups.

The authors also make the unsupported statement that “it is generally accepted that seasonal differences influence OA, with patients being worse in cold, damp and unstable weather.” A number of studies have found this traditionally assumed relationship to be difficult to verify and likely a minor and insignificant factor in arthritis pain for most patients (1, 2, 3), so it does not qualify as “generally accepted.” Nevertheless, the authors go on to claim that a trend observed of worsening symptoms for the placebo group during the treatment phase of the study and subsequent improvement during the post-treatment follow-up was due to the weather, and that the opposite trend seen in the Zeel and Carprofen groups was due to the effects of the treatment agents. It seems more likely that the placebo group simply different in significant ways from the other groups, which casts further doubt on the conclusion that the test product was of meaningful benefit.

The authors conclude by putting the usual best possible spin on the weak results, suggesting that combined with in vitro results reported elsewhere they justify further research and pointing out that NSAIDS, which even they acknowledge are clearly superior for treatment of pain, have side effects, despite the fact that none were seen in this study. As I’ve said before, the resource limitations on research in veterinary medicine requires the most efficient use of those resources to maximize benefit, and such studies of implausible interventions are not going to benefit our patients. The authors clearly wish to find something positive in their results, but the study does not justify the commitment of more time and resources and talent to this methodology when better therapies are already available, and when decades of research on homeopathic preparations has failed to validate them. Such papers provide the aura of scientific legitimacy to such methods, but they are tooth fairy science, not evidence-based medicine, and they are a dead end we would do well to stop travelling down.

Posted in Homeopathy | 9 Comments